View this article in Search Friendly Plain Text
NOTE: This plain text article interpretation has been digitally created by OCR software to estimate the article text, to help both users and search engines find relevant article content. To read the actual article text, view or download the PDF above.
SPIDER
COMMUNICATION
Mechanisms and Ecological Significance
SPIDER COMMUNICATION Mechanisms and Ecological Significance
In the last ten years aja-rge nuar-ber of published ra^^JhaBéi:-ported for the first time r^aits of the application of moderfSentific technology to the ^^^^ga-tion of spidp^BSsiology and behavior. ^Concentrating on tlle^^Bflex spider communication Jätern, this book assembles* the most regent multidisciplinary advances rJMi ing researchers |ir#m many tries^|B^j||| the peculiar role spiders pl^^M fht animal HHk Here ^P^K^iu’s* contributors Æ view their mp9fe^^|place them within the general tidft and speculate on the «vantages and disadvan-.J?#ges that special ffifelopment has provided for spiders.
Topics covered and their aÄiOjs are: significance and complexity of spidif communication ■(Bertrand Krafft); vibratory signals (Friedrich G. Barth); acouslB communication and reproductive isolationRI^sfaSäs W. Uetz and Gj|§ E. Sfratton); visual communication in jumping spiders (Lyn Forste» communication behavior in jumping spiders (RoM® R. Jackson); Hmaical communication in LycOsid| and other” (William J. Tietjen a® jferomBs. Rovher); social spacing strJ§|jffl (J. Wesley Burgess and George W. Uet^)|i»d foraging (Susan E. Riech-ert and fadwiga Euczak ).
Peter N. Witt w&fbrmerily ChdM^ il Mental Health Research, Depart-ment of Human ^ef9^Cû|âta$e of i^iprA Carolina.
Jerome S. Rovner is Professor of Zoology at Ohio Unw^as^B
Chapter 1
INTRODUCTION: COMMUNICATION IN SPIDERS
Peter N. Witt
North Carolina Mental Health Research Anderson Hall Dorothea Dix Hospital Raleigh, North Carolina 27611
Most of the authors of this book were invited by me to come together as part of the International Meeting sponsored by the American Arachnological Society in the summer of 1978 in Gainesville, Florida. They agreed to discuss their work as it contributed to our knowledge about communication in spiders. I conceived the plan for the book at the symposium and thus became its senior editor. At a later date I asked Jerome Rovner to join me in the editorial work, and we bear together the responsibility for the present list of contributors and the present jshape. The final version developed after the symposium through additional reviews of the relevant literature and inclusion of current laboratory and field work. All along we tried to preserve the attraction of the immediateness of the reports from the authors’ own laboratories and combine them with a more general review of the field. The text aims at presenting the present state of knowledge and is a compromise between completeness and readability. Sometimes a chapter fits only the widest definition of “communication,l^e.g., Riechert and Tuczak’s “Spider Foraging: Behavioral Responses to Prey.” Because it contains valuable information on spider behavior and ecology which is not to be found summarized elsewhere, and because it rounds off the other chapters by applying some of the sensory physiology and other knowledge to predator-prey interactions, the editors decided to include fi> After all, the subject matter of that chapter is an important part of the spider’s relationship with its environment. A special effort is made to address readers beyond arachnologists, so that they can sample and compare how much (or little) is known about one aspect of this one group of animals at the present time.
To many readers opening this volume, the title will seem strange, if not unworthy of serious and lengthy treatment. On the face of it, spiders do not deserve reputations for communicability. Rather, they are popularly regarded as solitary and silent predators, who neither seek, nor are likely to obtain, partners in any gentle exchange. A moment’s reflection will revise that judgment, but it is probably the first task of an introduction to offer some explanation of why the volume is thicker than one might anticipate. The events of courtship would probably be granted at once to be a matter of communication. Even the “news” which prey provides, however regretfully it does so, could be construed as communication. Indeed, this latter, very broad interpretation of the term “communication” is the one I have chosen to use in order to include material in this volume which is relevant to a consideration of mechanisms underlying communication sensu stiictu. Thus, even where the research dealt with a system for prey detection, the findings of such a study could also be of interest to those seeking to understand the use of the same channel for signaling by a male spider when courting a female.
For the reader who pictures only the solitary web-builder, there will be surprises in thfs book, especially ivhen communal spiders are considered. But the topic is capable of even wider expansion; So much so that in preparing this book one became convinced that to cover the problem thoroughly, it will never be thick enough. Scarcity of knowledge, rather than lack of subject matter worthy of discussion, has limited the size of the present work. In the search for knowledge, this book is a way station where we review how far we have come in order to determine where we want to go.
Invertebrates have recently become particularly interesting to neuroscientists. Chase (1979) points out that papers in invertebrate neurobiology constituted the third largest of 41 topical categories at the 1978 meeting, which represented the whole Society of Neuroscience’s interest. He discusses the advantages of relatively fewer neuronal elements for identification in invertebrates, together with greater technical accessibility. This accounts for the fact that physiological descriptions of behavioral control are more complete for invertebrate models than they are for vertebrate models. If one assumes evolutionary continuity from animals to man, it follows that studies of identification of the role of particular neurons in invertebrates become relevant to the human situation. This in turn permits more enlightened speculation on the mind-body problem (see Chase, 1979).
Spiders, one of several groups making up the class Arachnida, are invertebrates which show a number of peculiarities. An example of die special body structure of spiders is shown in Figure 1.1. Spiders show a number of precoded behavior patterns, frequently called fixed action patterns. In several places in this book such behavior
Figure 1.1a. Dorsal view of Aianeus diadematus, the cross spider, hanging face-down in its orb-web. Numbers 1, 2, 3, and 4 designate the first, second, third, and fourth right legs, respectively. P: pedipalp; CE: céphalothorax; A: abdomen; TA: tarsus of fourth left leg. Note that, in contrast to insects, the spider has a two-part body, four pairs of legs, and a pair of pedipalps instead of antennae. Figure 1.1b. Scanning electron micrograph of the ventral view of the same spider (face-down as in 1.1a). C: chelicera with fang; P: pedipalp; S: sternum (on the ventral side of céphalothorax); A: abdomen; CO: coxa; T: trochanter; F: femur of fourth leg.
patterns as courtship, web-building, and feeding will be described as highly ritualized and species-specific. The neuronal substrates of such behavior probably will be elucidated one day. The following chapters review much of the present knowledge as an early step in this process. Table 1.1 lists the families of spiders containing one or more species whose names will appear in these chapters.
Why do we focus on communication? Communication is important for an animal’s survival. Animals change their behavior as a consequence of the information they receive. In social animals all communal life is based on a communication network, which car-
Table 1.1. Families of spiders included in this book.
Web-weavers | Wanderers |
Agelenidae – funnel weavers | Anyphaenidae |
Amaurobiidae | Archaeidae |
Araneidae – “ordinary” orb-weavers | Clubionidae – sac spiders |
Argyronetidae – water spiders | Ctenidae |
Atypidae – purse-web spiders | Dysderidae |
Barychelidae | Gnaphosidae (Drassidaej ^ |
Ctenizidae – trapdoor spiders | Lycosidae – wolf spiders |
Dictynidae | Mimetidae – pirate spiders |
Diguetidae | Oxyopidae – lynx spiders |
Dinopidae – ogre-faced spiders | Philodromidae |
Dipluridae – funnel-web tarantulas | Pisauridae – nursery-web spiders |
(mygalomorphs) | Salticidae – jumping spiders |
Eresidae | Scytodidae – spitting spiders |
Filistatidae | Sicariidae |
Hahniidae | Sparassidae (Heteropodidae) – |
Hypochilidae | huntsman spiders |
Leptonetidae | Theraphosidae – “ordinary” |
Linyphiidae – sheet-web weavers | tarantulas (mygalomorphs) |
Liphistiidae | Thomisidae – crab spiders |
Loxoscelidae
Micryphantidae (Erigonidae) -dwarf spiders Ochyroceratidae Oecobiidae Pholcidae – long-legged spiders Segestriidae Tetragnathidae – long-jawed orb-weavers |
|
Theridiidae – comb-footed spiders
Uloboridae Urocteidae |
ries information among the individuals, letting each know what it has to do to assist in the survival of relatives and thereby insure its own best genetic interest. Communication between sexually reproducing animals is 6ne prerequisite for their genes’ survival.
There are many ways in which living beings communicate. Some of the ways are so characteristic for a species or genus that they can be used for defining the difference between that group and others. Humans are frequently distinguished from animals by the ability of our species to use language for communication. The more recent investigations of chimpanzees’ ability to master and apply American Sign Language are efforts to resolve the controversy over the degree of sophistication, abstraction, and generalization which these apes can develop in this communication medium as compared to humans fGriffin, 1977, 1978; Premack and Woodruff, 1978).
Understanding communication between living beings requires insight into many different aspects of life. The organs which transmit and receive signals can be studied as to their physical appearance and particular function. The signal itself—its variation, nature, means of transmittal, and its information content—is another subject for investigation. These components cannot be understood as part of the communication process if the meaning of the message is not clarified: how it was encoded by the communicator and how it was decoded and interpreted by the ieceiver. Usually, observation and the measurement of the individual’s behavior under specified conditions are used as methods for gaining some understanding of the content and meaning of a communication process.
The central position which communication plays in animals’ Slves can be deduced from the observation that studying communication leads to an understanding of the peculiarities of that animal and its conspecifics. The description of a simple experiment will illustrate the point.
A spider hangs face down in the center of its intricate orb web. A low-frequency tuning fork is struck. As soon as the vibrating prongs of the instrument touch a radius of the web, the spider turns and positions one front leg on the moving radius (Figure l|||). This trial can be repeated over and over, and the results will nearly always be the same. Further reaction is more variable and depends on a number of circumstances: the duration of the signal, the response of the web to short jerks on the radius by the first legs, the spider’s appetite, the number and character of preceding trials, and even general circumstances such as time of day, sun, wind, and rain.
What has just been described represents certain aspects of communication (broadly defined) which are characteristic of many species of spidêfcs. The tuning fork produced -a^measurable signal; frequency, intensity, and variability of the signal could have been tested for the ranges in which they elicit a response. The instrument was used in place; of another animal—a COn-specific or prey-8 to test the nature, frequency, and intensity of effective signal production. The signal was transmitted through a specific channel, in this case mechanically through the silken radial thread. The channel connected the signaler with the receiver: ThC resulting behavior of the spider, i.e., turningiin the direction of the tuning fork, provided evidence that the signal wks received and decoded. Äe lyri-form and slit organs on the legs of the spider (here Araneüß diäde-matus) hayi been shown in öther spiders to be sensitive to vibration-induced strains in the exöskeleton, as wilb be^discussed by Barth. These receptors ëërit impulses (action potentials) along nerve fibers through the legs to tS central nervous systëüî. Here the message was decoded and translated into outgoing nerve-borne signals, which resulted in patterned muscular contractions that produced movements of the legs and body. All sections of this communication system worked together to produce an observable ac-tion-reactiw sequence. As will be fully discussed in later chapters, fhf” investigator can analyze? the various parts of the system, and define the role each plays in the total pitleysdÄ Beyond the general conception of communication whigl We have derivèd from this observation, it has taught us much about the animal in its living space. This particular spider, like many of its relatives, has made qse of #lpfecific-‘ signal quality, namely the vj-bration of the substrate oii which it rests. Each type of signal, be it chemical, acoustic, visual or, as in this case, vibratory, has propertied which make it practical for a specific environment. Vibration, for example, is independent of light and can be as effective at night as during the day. It is relatively independent of air currents, which can, on the other hand, carry chemicals with~#em. Nocturnal spiders, which build an invisible trap to catch visually orienting flies, were preadapted with a sensory system which is highly receptive, independent of vision. Many spiders have compensated for the absence of a suitable substrate over which a vibratory signal can be conducted, as well as for limitations in the distance of conductMh in available substrates, extending the perceptual range of the legs with a silken structure. It is not enough that the area of capture is enlarged; the fact üf contact by the prey with the silk must be communicated.. Suddenly the radiating shape of the web (Figure 1.2) takes on a new meaning for the observer: the web extends the perceptual range of the sense of vibration from about 15mm to more than 500 mm bf-forming a suitable substrate for the transmission of vibrations to the legs. The legs, in turn, improve reception by pulling the silk tight.
Knowledge of the organs for communication letsms understand other behaviors of the animal. During orb-web construction, the spider pulls and probes existing threads to gather information on the degree of completion, before new strands are laid (Peters, 1938| Reed, 1969). It straddles angles, apparently assessing their width and comparing them to an internal “plan” of template, which guides web construction. In the laboratory we have let the cros^ spider build an orb-web in a closed box in complete darkness. Elaborate measurements carried out on the resulting webs showed no differences between these webs and those built by the same animal in partial light. No longer dependent on vision, and in darkness safe from visually searching predators, the spider builds an almost invisible web on whifh if will catch visually orienting prey.
Pertinent to the topic of communication is the fact that the web also plays a delicate and essential r©|e in the spider’s courtship behavior, A male may drum or pluck on the web ©f a female over long periods of time, until the aggressive attack of the female changes to acceptance of the male for copulation. It can be shown, by comparison of the webs of a few spider species (Risch, 1977), that the web built by the adult female is more species-specific jhan is the juvenile web. This observation holds for measures of size, fine structure, and shape. It raises the question of whether the specific resonance of the female web plays a role in Species recognition for the “short-sighted” male. Blanke’s (1973) experiments revealed reactions of males to wind which had blown across sexually mature females, suggesting that in finding the female web the male may be aided by an airborne signal, probably a chemical. This may then be followed by vibratory communication. Rarely is only one sensory modality involved in communication behavto#; JgBtead, one channel is usually predominant and others assist in one stage communication. The roles may be reversed in the next stage.
Jt is not necessary to suppose that the spider has any conception of the world which she so nicely manages. In this respect, the condition bears some resemblance to the human condition. The world outside us, and the mental picture we have of it, is a product of what we can detect through our receptor organs and the subsequent analyses in the brain. We s,ee colors and shades from which we guess at shapes and materials. We smell, feel, hear; and we use those inputs to recognize, categorize, understand objects and other living beings. Philosophers like Schopenhauer have gone so far as to describe the world as a product of our Will and imagination.
Arthur Schopenhauer’s principal work is the book Die Welt als Wille und Vorstellung, which appeared first in Leipzig in 1819, and was translated into English in 1883 under the title The World âà Will and Idea (see ref. Schopenhauer). The second part of the title, which describes the first part of his philosophy, is OfJ special interest here. The German word*Vorstellung can also be translated as “representation, conception, mental image”r (The New Cassell’s Dictionary, 1958). Any of those three words appear to this writer to describe part of Schopenhauer’s philosophy better than the common translation “idea.” Based on Descartes, Kant, and Locke, Schopenhauer stressed the distinction between the phenomenon, or the appearance that a thing presents to the perceiving mind, and the thing as it is in itself. Through perception the mind is presumed to be aware only of the observable facts or events, i.e., of the phenomena: what lies behind them, being beyond all possible experiertë; is unknowable. It is impossible to dissociate conceptual thinking from the perceptual experience on which it is based. To use Schopenhauer’s words (in translation): “Conceptions and abstractions which do not ultimately refer to perception [translator’s italics] are like paths in the wood that end without leading out of i|P In brief, our conception or understanding of the world informed by the organs we have to perceive it; thus, we have a predominantly visual world. A very different world, mainly filled with touch and vibration signals, exists for the web-building spider. By discussing the spider’s organs for communication, some of the contributors to this book try to introduce the reader to the “inner world” of the spider.
Such an argument is not intended to mean that the present author believes exclusively in the familiar “idea that there can be nothing in our intellect which has not entered it through our senses.” We must assume that “every animal is born with expectations and anticipations” (Popper, 1974), which means that it possesses inborn knowledge. However, it is argued that observation of the perceptual repertoire of an animal (including the central nervous processing of signals) introduces some special understanding of the animal and its works, in our case of spiders, which cannot be gained otherwise.
So far the word “communication’ ‘ has been used loosely; no definition Èês been given! We have talked about communication between living beings, thereby excluding communication processes iftMde one being, i.e., nervous or chemical signals which carry messages from one part of the body to another. We have excluded passive communication between an individual and its environment, i.e., the perception of and reaction to temperature changes can sometimes be called passive communication (E. O. Wilson, 197® Internal messages and the impact of the environment will only be discussed so far as they affect communication processes between animals. There are many definitions of communication available {see Smith, 1977; Sebeok||jl9^; Wilson, 1975^ Each definition serve,s a specific purpose; they-, are nibt mutually exclusive, but rather complementary. For that reason, none has been sinf$|à out. Rather it has been left to the authors of the different chapters jt>o choose the definition which best suits their approach to the overall theme of communication in spiders.
Special signals carry messages for communication (for exceptions see Smith, 1977, p. 13). Such signals have to be produced by an organ which is specially adapted fiffflsignal production. A number of conditions have to be fulfulled before a signal is actually sent] out: the physiological stage of the signaling organism has to B right before the environment, (or the partner) can elicit the signal. Both the state of the organism and the avironmental releaser determine whether a signal is sent.
Some of the Slowing chapters focus attention on the nature of the ,signal, i.e., they discuss airborne, chemical, and other modes of communication. In pulsed signals, for example, one may distinguish between frequency and intensity, find graded and stereotyped repertoires, and note the manner in which the beginning and end of the signal are determined by the signaler or the environment. However, very few messages depend on one channel of communication only. In humans the visual perception of the signaler’s face joins with the tactile message of a handshake and.tl^auditory message of a greeting to communicate “welcome/’The quality of any one of these elements, or its absence, may radically alter the message. The problems raised by the spectrum of signals and their possible combinations require additional kinds of study, and are the concern of other chapfcy^^
The message can only get across to another lining being if that individual has an organ sensitif to the nature of the signal, a way to decode the message, and is ready Jo receive it. .The study of receptor organs—their sensitivity and ability fi discriminate—is combined with observation of the behavior o| ÊÈÊ receiver to reveal whether a message was received and what its significance was. Analysis of communication behavior requires a special line of investigation, at a level of analysis which is just as important for our understanding of communication as are the anatomy and physiology of the communicating organisms and the physics of signal production, transmission, and reception.
Another way to lookët communication is to place it in the wider context of its contribution to the survival of the organism, i.ë., to gauge its adaptive value (Burghardt, 1977). It is generally assumed that organisms which live communally, frequently in structured societies, use more and a greater variety of signals than do solitary animals. However, an animal which lives alone is still dependent on communication with conspecifics (and sometimes with animals of other species). One has to assume that the lonely spide¥ of our example, in the middle of its orb-web, is at leist occasionally interacting with other orb-web builders with whom she competes for prey.
Communication sÿstems: change throughout the lifetime of an animal as requirements to communicate differ. The tiny immature spiderling, its nutritional nëéâs supplied By the yolk, probably has communication requirements different from those of the adult female, one hundred times heavier, sexually mature, and a voracious feeder (Burch, 1979). Such ontogenetic development in the communication system of spiders has been little studied, an exception being Aspey’s (197Sfpstudy of ontogeny of display in a #blf spider. On the other hand, a type of communication like thewlsual signaling given and received by jumping spiders may vary from species to closely related species. Comparison of communication systems can assist in tracing the phylogenÿfèf^whole taxonomic groups of spiders.
Communication involves at least two, and frequently more than two animals. If there is no receiver for a message sent out by a display of an animal, communication cannot take place. There is always a mutual evolutionary advantage (Smith, 1977)” involved in successful communicat-feii.1 Frequently it achie^if^a central social function. One can assume that the degree of social organization of a species can be measured by its use of intraspecific communication.
Filially, one may ask whether spiders have developed ways specific to them iM’which they communicate? Do their communication systems set them apart from other animals? In many, the production and daily usé of silk has opened up for them a means of communication which only few other animals can rival. When some of that knowledge was reviewed several years ago, Witt (1975), focusing on the orb-web, found that a small amount of evidence went together with a large array of speculation and surmise. Much has been discovered since that time, and some chapters in this book update our knowledge of various aspects of spider communication.
The reader may be convinced by now that there is a good case for bringing together much of the existing knowledge about this topic. By reviewing communication in spiders—the organs involved, the purposes which communication serves, the circumstances under which it takes place, and the effect it has—we will increase our understanding of the more than 30,000 species of spiders (Levi and Levi, 1968) as much as we would by a review of their size, shape, and color. One may even be convinced that several authors are necessary to bring all the pieces of knowledge together in order to make it possible to understand fully the peculiarities of spiders and the relationship of species with each other. Platnick (1971) stresses that stereotyped patterns (e.g., courtship behavior) must be considered at least as important a character for systematics as morpholi ogy. But one can still ask whether the beautifully illustrated large books, like the recent volume by Gertsch (1979) on American Spiders, do not sufficiently cover the subject. The material presented in our book is so different from that discussed in other “spider books“ (see references) that it can serve as a complement to these without repeating any of their details. Indeed, only the ten-page review by Weygoldt (1977) on “Communication in Crustaceans and Arachnids“ has tried anything similar to this book and did so, of necessity, in a very much shorter form. Ours is the first book of its kind, one which discusses a specific aspect of all spiders’ lives, and so increases our knowledge of this interesting group of animals.
Acknowledgments
The author’s more than thirty years of web analysis, which led to the organization of this book and the insights discussed in the Introduction, was for many years supported by the Swiss National Fund, the National Science Foundation, and many co-workers, whose names appear in the reference lists. Mrs. R. Daniels worked untiringly on the arrangement and retyping of manuscripts. Dr. C. F. Reed provided invaluable stimulation and advice.